Multiple Intelligences After Twenty Years, ©Howard Gardner 2003
Multiple Intelligences After Twenty
Years
By Howard Gardner
©Howard Gardner 2003
Address for Correspondence:
Howard Gardner Harvard
Graduate School of Education
Larsen Hall, Room 201 Appian Way
Cambridge, MA 02138
Phone: (617) 496-4929 Email: howard@pz.harvard.edu
Paper presented at the American
Educational Research Association, Chicago, Illinois, April 21, 2003.
Multiple Intelligences After Twenty
Years
Howard Gardner Harvard Graduate
School of Education
Invited Address, American Educational
Research Association
April 21, 2003
© Howard Gardner, 2003
I am often asked how I first got the
idea of the theory of multiple intelligences. Probably the most truthful answer
is “I don’t know”. However, such an answer satisfies neither the questioner
nor, to be frank, me. With the benefit of hindsight, I would mention the
following distal and proximal factors:
1.
As
a young person I was a serious pianist and enthusiastically involved with other
arts as well. When I began to study developmental and cognitive psychology, I
was struck by the virtual absence of any mention of the arts. An early
professional goal was to find a place for the arts within academic psychology.
I am still trying! In 1967 my continuing interest in the arts prompted me to
become a founding member of Project Zero, a basic research group at the Harvard
Graduate School of Education begun by a noted philosopher of art, Nelson
Goodman. For 28 years, I was the co-director of Project Zero and I am happy to
say that the organization continues to thrive.
2.
As
my doctoral career was drawing to a close, I first encountered the neurological
research of Norman Geschwind. I was fascinated by Geschwind’s discussion of
what happens to once normal or gifted individuals who have the misfortune of
suffering from a stroke or some other form of brain damage. Often the symptoms
run counter to intuition: for example, a patient who is alexic but not agraphic
loses the ability to read words but can still read numbers, name objects, and
write normally. Without planning it that way, I ended up working for twenty
years on a neuropsychological unit, trying to understand the organization of
human abilities in the brain.
3.
I
have always enjoyed writing and by the time I began my postdoctoral work with
Geschwind, I had completed three books. My fourth book, The Shattered Mind,
published in 1975, chronicled what happens to individuals who suffer from
different forms of brain damage. I documented how different parts of the brain
are dominant for different cognitive functions. After I completed The Shattered
Mind, I thought that I might write a book that describes the psychology of
different human faculties—a modern version of phrenology. In 1976 I actually
wrote an outline for a book with the tentative title Kinds of Minds. One could
say that this book was never written—and indeed I had forgotten about it for
many years. But one could also say that it eventually arose from the file cabinet
and became Frames of Mind.
So much for
the distal causes of the theory.
In 1979, a
group of researchers affiliated with the Harvard Graduate School of Education
received a sizeable grant from a Dutch foundation, the Bernard Van Leer
Foundation. This grant was designed for a grandiose purpose, one proposed by the
foundation. Members of the Project on Human Potential (as it came to be called)
were expected to carry out scholarly work on the nature of human potential and
how it could best be catalyzed. When we carved out our respective projects, I
received an interesting assignment: to write a book about what had been
established about human cognition through discoveries in the biological and
behavioral sciences. Thus was born the research program that led to the theory
of multiple intelligences. Support from the Van Leer Foundation allowed me to
carry out an extensive research program with the aid of many younger
colleagues. I saw this as a once in a lifetime opportunity to collate and
synthesize what I and others had learned about the development of cognitive capacities
in normal and gifted children as well as the breakdown of such capacities in
individuals who suffered some form of pathology. To put it in terms of my daily
calendar, I was seeking to synthesize what I was learning in the morning from
my study of brain damage with what I was learning in the afternoon from my
study of cognitive development. My colleagues and I combed the literature from
brain study, genetics, anthropology, and psychology in an effort to ascertain
the optimal taxonomy of human capacities. I can identify a number of crucial
turning points in this investigation. I don’t remember when it happened but at
a certain moment, I decided to call these faculties “multiple intelligences”
rather than abilities or gifts. This seemingly minor lexical substitution
proved very important; I am quite confident that if I had written a book called
“Seven Talents” it would not have received the attention that Frames of Mind
received. As my colleague David Feldman has pointed out, the selection of this
word placed me in direct confrontation with the psychological establishment
that cherishes IQ tests. However, I disagree with Feldman’s claim that I was
motivated by a desire to “slay IQ”; neither the documentary nor the mnemonic
evidence suggests to me that I had much interest in such a confrontation. A
second crucial point was the creation of a definition of an intelligence and
the identification of a set of criteria that define what is, and what is not,
an intelligence. I can’t pretend that the criteria were all established a
priori; rather, there was a constant fitting and refitting of what I was
learning about human abilities with how best to delineate what ultimately
became 8 criteria. I feel that the definition and the criteria are among the
most original parts of the work; but neither has received much discussion in
the literature. When I began the book, I was writing as a psychologist and that
is still my primary scholarly identification. Yet, given the mission of the Van
Leer Foundation, it was clear to me that I needed to say something about the
educational implications of MI theory. And so, I conducted some research on
education and touched on some educational implications of the theory in the
concluding chapters. This decision turned out to be another crucial point
because it was educators, rather than psychologists, who found the theory of
most interest. By 1981 I had drafted the book; thereafter I worked on
revisions. The main lines of the argument had become clear. I was claiming that
all human beings possess not just a single intelligence (often called “g” for
general intelligence). Rather, as a species we human beings are better
described as having a set of relatively autonomous intelligences. Most lay and
scholarly writings about intelligence focus on a combination of linguistic and
logical intelligences—the intellectual strengths, I often maintain, of a law
professor. However, a fuller appreciation of human beings occurs if we take
into account spatial, bodily-kinesthetic, musical, interpersonal, and
intrapersonal intelligences. While we all have these intelligences, individuals
differ for both genetic and experiential reasons in their respective profiles
of intellectual strengths and weaknesses. No intelligence is in and of itself
artistic or non-artistic; rather several intelligences can be put to aesthetic
ends, if individuals so desire. No direct educational implications follow from
this psychological theory; but if individuals differ in their intellectual
profiles, it makes sense to take this fact into account in devising an
educational system. By the time that Frames of Mind was published in 1983, I
had already published half a dozen books. Each had had a modestly positive
reception and a reasonable sale. I did not expect anything different from
Frames of Mind, a lengthy and (for a trade audience) somewhat technical book.
But within a few months after its publication, I realized that this book was
different. Not that the reviews were that exuberant or the sales that
monumental. Rather, there was genuine “buzz” about the book. I got invited to
give many talks, and when I showed up at a site, people had at least heard
about the theory and were eager to learn more about it. I sometimes quip that
“MI theory” gave me my fifteen minutes of fame. While I have done many things
in my professional life, I realize that I am likely always to be known as the
“father of multiple intelligences” or, less palatably, as the “MI guru.” (I
remember with some vividness an appearance at AERA in the mid-1980s where I
described the theory to a packed auditorium. My friend Bob Sternberg introduced
me, and, according to one observer, spoke for 22 minutes!) For the first decade
following the publication of Frames of Mind, I had two primary relations to the
theory. The first relationship was that of a bemused observer. I was amazed at
how many individuals said that they wanted to revise their educational
practices in the light of MI theory. Within a year or so I had already met with
the teachers from Indianapolis who would shortly begin the Key School, the
first school in the world organized explicitly around MI theory. I began to
receive a steady stream of communications asking or telling me how to use MI
theory in various kinds of schools or for various populations. While I tried to
be responsive to these communications, I always maintained that I was a
psychologist and not an educator, and did not presume to know how best to teach
a class of young persons or run an elementary or secondary school. My second
relation was as a director of research projects that grew out of MI theory. The
most ambitious effort was Project Spectrum, a collaboration with David Feldman,
Mara Krechevsky, Janet Stork, and others. The goal of Project Spectrum was to
create a set of measures whereby one could ascertain the intellectual profile
of young children— preschoolers and those in the primary grades. We ended up
devising fifteen separate tasks that were designed to assess the several
intelligences in as natural a manner as possible. We had a great deal of fun
devising the Spectrum battery and using it with different populations. We also
learned that creating assessments is a difficult task and one that requires a
great investment of money and time. I decided, without saying so in so many
words, that I did not want myself to be in the assessment business, though I
was very pleased if others chose to create instruments in an effort to assess
the various intelligences. Let me mention a few other research projects that
grew out of the first wave of interest in MI theory. Working with Robert
Sternberg of Yale, another critic of standard views of intelligence, my
colleagues and I created a middle school curriculum called Practical
Intelligences for School. Working with colleagues from the Educational Testing
Service, my colleagues and I developed a set of curriculum-and-assessment
instruments designed to document learning in three art forms. There were also
collaborative efforts in the use of computers in education. To my surprise and
pleasure, interest in multiple intelligences survived the transition to the
1990s. By that time, I was prepared to undertake several new activities. The
first was purely scholarly. Building on the notion of different kinds of
intelligences, I carried out case studies of individuals who stood out as
remarkable in terms of their particular profile of intelligences. This line of
work led to my books on creativity (Creating Minds), leadership (Leading
Minds), and extraordinary achievement, more broadly (Extraordinary Minds). You
can see that I was getting a lot of mileage by injecting book titles with the
term ‘mind’! The second was an extension of the theory. In 1994-5 I took a
sabbatical and used part of that time to review evidence for the existence of
new intelligences. I concluded that there was ample evidence for a naturalist
intelligence; and suggestive evidence as well for a possible existential
intelligence (“the intelligence of big questions”). I also explored much more
deeply the relation between intelligences—which I construe as biopsychological
potentials—and the various domains and disciplines that exist in various
cultures. What we know and how we parse the world may well be in part a
reflection of the human intelligences. I also introduced three distinct uses of
the term “intelligence”:
* A property
of all human beings (All of us possess these 8 or 9 intelligences)
* A dimension
on which human beings differ (No two people—not even identical twins— possess
exactly the same profile of intelligences)
* The way in
which one carries out a task in virtue of one’s goals (Joe may have a lot of
musical intelligence but his interpretation of that piece made little sense to
us)
A third
activity featured a more proactive relationship to the uses and interpretations
of my theory. For the first decade, I had been content simply to observe what
others were doing and saying in the name of MI theory. But by the middle 1990s,
I had noticed a number of misinterpretations of the theory—for example, the
confusion of intelligences with learning styles and the confounding of a human
intelligence with a societal domain (e.g. musical intelligence being equated
with mastery of a certain musical genre or role). I had also taken note of
practices that I found offensive—for example, describing different racial or
ethnic group in terms of their characteristic intelligences. And so, for the
first time, I began to differentiate my “take” on MI from that of others who
had learned about and tried to make use of the theory. A final feature of this
second phase entailed a more active involvement with educational reform. This
involvement took both a practical and a scholarly form. On the practical level,
my colleagues and I at Harvard Project Zero began working with schools as they
attempted to implement MI practices and other educational programs that we have
developed, such as teaching for understanding. We also launched a Summer
Institute which is now in its 7th year. On the scholarly side, I began to
articulate my own educational philosophy. In particular, I focussed on the
importance in the precollegiate years of achieving understanding in the major
disciplines—science, mathematics, history, and the arts. For various reasons,
achieving such understanding is quite challenging. Efforts to cover too much
material doom the achievement of understanding. We are most likely to enhance
understanding if we probe deeply in a small number of topics. And once the
decision is made to “uncover” rather than “cover,” it is possible to take
advantage of our multiple intelligences. Put concretely, we can approach topics
in a number of ways; we can make use of analogies and comparisons drawn from a
range of domains; and we can express the key notions or concepts in a number of
different symbolic forms. This analysis has led to a perhaps surprising
conclusion. “Multiple intelligences” should not in and of itself be an
educational goal. Educational goals need to reflect one’s own values, and these
can never come simply or directly from a scientific theory. Once one reflects
on one’s educational values and states one’s educational goals, however, then
the putative existence of our multiple intelligences can prove very helpful.
And, in particular, if one’s educational goals encompass disciplinary
understanding, then it is possible to mobilize our several intelligences to
help achieve that lofty goal. This, then, is how the first twenty years of
multiple intelligences look to me. I am grateful to the many individuals who
have taken an interest in the theory—both within my research group and across
the country and the globe. I have tried to be responsive to their inquiries and
to build on what they have taught me. And I have come to realize that once one
releases an idea—a “meme”—into the world, one cannot completely control its
behavior—anymore than one can control those products of our genes called
children. Put succinctly, MI has and will have a life of its own, over and
above what I might wish for it, my most widely known intellectual offspring. MI
turns 20 in the same year that I turn 60. I do not know how much time I will
have left to work on the theory, nor can I claim that the theory occupies the
majority of my attention any longer. But this moment is an excellent one for me
to step back and to suggest some future lines of analysis and practice. To
begin with, there will be efforts to propose new intelligences. In recent
years, in addition to the explosion of interest in emotional intelligences,
there have also been serious efforts to describe a spiritual intelligence and a
sexual intelligence. My colleague Antonio Battro has proposed the existence of
a digital intelligence and has indicated how it may fulfill the criteria that I
have set forth. And at this conference, Michael Posner has challenged me to
consider “attention” as a kind of intelligence. I have always conceded that, in
the end, the decision about what counts as an intelligence is a judgment call
and not an algorithmic conclusion. So far, I am sticking to my 8 l/2
intelligences but I can readily foresee a time when the list could grow, or
when the boundaries among the intelligences might be reconfigured. For example,
to the extent that the so-called Mozart effect gains credibility, I might want
to rethink the relation between musical and spatial intelligences. Much work
needs to be done on the question of how the intelligences can best be mobilized
to achieve specific pedagogical goals. I do not believe that educational
programs created under the aegis of MI theory lend themselves to the kinds of
randomized control studies that the federal government is now calling for in
education. But I do believe that well choreographed “design experiments” can
reveal the kinds of educational endeavors where an MI perspective is
appropriate and where it is not. To state just one example, I think that MI
approaches are particularly useful when a student is trying to master a
challenging new concept—say, gravity in physics, or the Zeitgeist in history. I
am less persuaded that it can be useful in mastering a foreign language—though
I admire those teachers of foreign languages who claim success using MI
approaches. Were I to be granted more time and energy to explore the
ramifications of MI theory, I would devote those precious gifts to two
endeavors. First of all, as indicated above, I have become increasingly
fascinated by the ways in which societal activities and domains of knowledge
emerge and become periodically reconfigured. Any complex society has 100- 200
distinct occupations at the least; and any university of size offers at least
50 different areas of study. Surely these domains and disciplines are not
accidents; nor are the ways that they evolve and combine random events. The
culturally-constructed spheres of knowledge must bear some kind of relation to
the kinds of brains and minds that human beings have, and the ways that those
brains and minds grow and develop in different cultural settings. Put
concretely, how does human logical-mathematical intelligence relate to the
various sciences, mathematics, and computing software and hardware that have
emerged in the last few thousand years, and those that may emerge one year or
100 years from now? Which makes which or, more probably, how does each shape
the other? How does the human mind deal with interdisciplinary studies—are they
natural or unnatural cognitive activities? I would love to be able to think
about these issues in a systematic way. Second, from the start, one of the
appealing aspects of MI theory was its reliance on biological evidence. At the
time, in the early 1980s, there was little relevant evidence from genetics or
evolutionary psychology; such speculations were mere handwaving. There was
powerful evidence from the study of neuropsychology for the existence of
different mental faculties; and that evidence constituted the strongest leg on
which to justify MI theory. Twenty years later, knowledge is accumulating at a
phenomenal rate in both brain science and genetics. At the risk of seeming
hyperbolic, I am prepared to defend the proposition that we have learned as
much from 1983 to 2003 as we did in the previous 500 years. As an amateur
geneticist and neuroscientist, I have tried as best I can to keep up with the
cascade of new findings from these areas. I can say with some confidence that
no findings have radically called into question the major lines of MI theory.
But I can say with equal confidence that in light of the findings of the last
two decades, the biological basis of MI theory needs urgently to be brought up
to date. Whether I will be in a position to do this myself, I cannot say. But I
would like to throw out a speculation. At the time that MI theory was
introduced, it was very important to make the case that human brains and human
minds are highly differentiated entities. It is fundamentally misleading to
think about a single mind, a single intelligence, a single problem-solving
capacity. And so, along with many others, I tried to make the argument that the
mind/brain consists of many modules/organs/intelligences, each of which
operates according to its own rules in relative autonomy from the others.
Happily, nowadays, the argument for modularity has been well made. Even those
who believe strongly in ‘general intelligence’ and/or neural plasticity feel
the need to defend their position, in a way that was unnecessary in decades
past. But it is time to revisit the issue of the relationship between general
and particular intelligences. This revisiting can and is being done in various
intriguing ways. Psychologist Robbie Case proposed the notion of central
conceptual structures—broader than specific intelligences but not as
all-encompassing as Piagetian general intelligence. Philosopher Jerry Fodor
contrasts impenetrable dedicated modules with a permeable central system. The
team of Marc Hauser, Noam Chomsky, and Tecumseh Fitch suggests that the unique
quality of human cognition is its capacity for recursive thinking; perhaps it
is recursion that characterizes advanced thinking in language, number, music,
social relations, and other realms. Electrophysiological and radiological
studies indicate that various brain modules may already be activated in
newborns. Neural imaging studies of individuals solving IQ-style problems
suggest that certain areas of the brain are most likely to be drawn on for
these kinds of problems; and there may be evidence for genes that contribute to
unusually high IQ, as there clearly are genes that cause retardation. And our
own case studies of unusually high performances suggests a distinction between
those who (like musicians or mathematicians) are outstanding in one area, as
opposed to those generalists (politicians or business leaders) who display a
relatively flat profile of cognitive strengths. Were I granted another lifetime
or two, I would like to rethink the nature of intelligence with respect to our
new biological knowledge, on the one hand, and our most sophisticated
understanding of the terrain of knowledge and societal practice, on the
other—another Van Leer Project on Human Potential, perhaps! I don’t expect this
wish to be granted. But I am glad to have had the chance to make an opening
move some twenty years ago; to have been able to revisit the gameboard
periodically; and to lay out this problematic so that other interested players
can have their chance to engage.
Σχόλια
Δημοσίευση σχολίου