Empirical Support for the Theory of Multiple Intelligences
The
Neuroscience of Intelligence: Empirical Support for the Theory of Multiple
Intelligences?
C. Branton Shearer1 and
Jessica M. Karanian2
1MI Research and Consulting
2 Department of Psychology,
Boston College
Corresponding
Author:
C. Branton
Shearer
1316 S. Lincoln
St.
Kent, OH 44240
Tel.: (330)
687-1735
C. Branton
Shearer is the creator of the Multiple Intelligences Developmental Assessment
Scales.
This research
did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public,
commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Key Words: intelligence, multiple intelligences,
cognition, general intelligence, neural correlates
Abstract
The
concept of intelligence has been strongly debated since introduction of IQ
tests in the 1900s. Numerous alternatives to unitary intelligence have achieved
limited acceptance by both psychologists and educators. Multiple intelligences
theory (Gardner, H. (1983,1993). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York:
Basic Books), despite criticism that it lacks
empirical validity, has had sustained interest by educators worldwide. MI
theory was one of the first to be based on neuroscience evidence. This
investigation reviewed 318 neuroscience reports to conclude that there is robust
evidence that each intelligence possesses neural coherence comparable with
general intelligence. Implications for using MI theory as a bridge between
instruction and cognitive neuroscience are discussed.
The concept of intelligence has a
checkered history in the minds of many scientists and educational theorists.
Many have abandoned the concept in part or entirely, and instead investigate
cognitive abilities, problem-solving, or information processing capacities.
However, many scientists have also investigated the functional neural systems that
underlie intellectual achievement. The reason for this has been summed up
succinctly by Jung and Haier [1, p. 171] “...there is no more important concept
in education than the concept of intelligence”They assert that not all brains
think the same way, thus “this simple fact could be revolutionary for education
because it demands a neuroscience approach that recognizes the importance of
individual differences and the necessity to evaluate each student as an
individual” [2, p. 174].
The
theory of multiple intelligences (MI) is of primary interest to the present
investigation. Howard Gardner [3,4]redefined intelligence as the ability to
solve problems or create products of value in a culture or community.Using this
broad, common sense definition and eight criteria* that cover a range of
evidence (e.g., neuroscience, workplace behaviors, great cultural achievements),
Gardner identified eight distinct forms of intelligence that are possessed by
all people, but in varying degrees. The eight intelligences identified are
linguistic, logical-mathematical, spatial, kinesthetic, musical, interpersonal,
intrapersonal and naturalist (for detailed descriptions, see Appendix A).
Traditional
psychologists criticize MI theory for a number of reasons. One criticism is
that MI theory lacks support from large scale studies [4,5] or experimental
research[7,8,9. It has also been proposed that the eight intelligences are
simply different manifestations of general intelligence [10,11]. An important
practical criticism is that educators should not base instructional and
curricular decisions upon a theory that lacks support from neuroscience
evidence [12,13] and is unsubstantiated and unproven[14,15,16].
Amongneuroscientists,
the predominant view on intelligence is that there iseither one general
intelligence (g) or two types of
intelligence (fluid and crystallized). However, there is a debate regardingthepossible
sub-divisions of intelligence and each sub-division’s relationship to “g”. Numerous other theories that deviate
from the unitary intelligence theory – includingtriarchic[17], emotional intelligence
[18,19], structure of intellect [20], faculties of mind [21], and cognitive
styles [22] – have had noteworthy, but limited, influence. Many have been
recognized by the field of psychology, but not embraced by educators. Few have
had the lasting and profound impact on education as multiple intelligences theory
which is still of interest world-wide more than 30 years after its introduction
[3, 4, 23]. Despite this broad appeal to educators, MI remains more of an
inspirational educational framework rather than a fully developed scientific
theory [24, 25, 2].
The
practical critiques are of particular importance as the emerging field of
educational cognitive neuroscience strives to establish a foundation for neuroscientific
evidence-based instructional approaches. This new field has struggled to build
practical connections between brain activity and instruction/curriculum. In its
early years, there was widespread skepticism that brain-based education could
develop without an explicit use of psycho-educational theory to bridge between
neuronal activity and instruction [26]. This situation has improved more
recently [27, 28, 29, 30], but the field continues to struggle to make a
distinction between “pop psychology” of brain-based teaching and the science of
educational cognitive neuroscience that can be systematically applied.
(Table 1 here)
The
following literature review organizes 30 years of cognitive neuroscienceresearch
on human cognition into core cognitive units that are each associated with a
particular intelligence. We compared theneuroscientific evidence for each
intelligence to the cortical areas outlined byGardner [3 ,4](Table 1) to
address the following inter-related questions: (1) do these neural functional
structures and networks display shared coherence while being conceptually
unique and distinct from other functions, (2) taken together, do these data
describe a solid conceptual framework for the “neural architecture” underlying
each of the eight intelligences, and (3) how well do these neural architectures
compare to what is known about the neural basis for general intelligence (i.e.,
g theory)? It should be underscored that this review of the cognitive
neuroscience literature in relation to MI theory is intended to provide a
foundation rather than a definitive examination of the constantly evolving
literature on the neural underpinnings of human cognition.
Methods
Procedures
This
investigation began with a detailed review of the various cognitive units and
specific skills associated with each intelligence. For example, musical intelligence
includes instrumental, vocal, composing and appreciation. Each of these ability
sets includes technical skill as well as creative performance (e.g., singing on
key and jazz improvisation) so the review of musical neuroscience studies would
ideally be inclusive of this range of abilities. Charts were constructed for
each intelligence with rows for MI Cognitive Units and columns for matched
Neural Structures and Cognitive Skills (linguistic sample in Appendix B. All
data is available upon request).
Using
the terms related to each Cognitive Unit or specific skill (Table 2), PubMed or
Google Scholar were used to search for published peer-reviewed empirical
neuroscience studies (neural organization Appendix C and journals list in
Appendix D). The goal was to identify a minimum of three to five studies per
major skill area. Surprisingly, a great many more studies were obtained. Studies
of personality characteristics or dispositions were not included (e.g.,
introversion, diligence, etc.). Theoretical articles or books were used mainly
for background information. Several extensive meta-analysis and topic reviews
served as guides to finding pertinent studies in the target area. Over 318
articles were referenced for the eight intelligences. The minimum number of
studies was 19 for Logical-mathematical with a maximum of 73 for Intrapersonal
(Table 2).
(Table 2 here)
From
this wealth of knowledge excerpts from each text describing neural activations associated
with carefully defined cognitive skills were entered into the charts per
Cognitive Unit(see linguistic sample in Appendix B and E). As the investigation
proceeded, the labels and defining characteristics for various Cognitive Units
were adjusted to better align the neuroscience evidence with MI theory (Table 2,
columns 6 and 7). This became a dialectical process between compatible
perspectives. The next step was for an objective neuroscience doctoral student
to review the data charts and harmonize the various neural descriptors
according to standard neural anatomical terminology. All neural regions were
then put into an Excel spreadsheet and reorganized based on neural hierarchy
(Appendices C and E).
It
became a challenge to manage the varieties of neural terminology.
Neuroscientific researchers have used a wide variety of terms and labels and
specificity over the years as the technology has evolved. Some researchers
identified broad regions with a single label while others used multiple terms
to identify sub-regions. Still others used Brodmann numbering,TalairachAtlas or
the MNI Coordinate system. This variety of nomenclaturesrequired a careful translation
and mapping onto the three-level hierarchy (Primary, sub-regions and particular
structures) described below.
Our
analysis of this data employed both qualitative and quantitative methods to
determine if a three-dimensional view of the neural structures associated with
each intelligence could be created. This hybrid approach – qualitative and
quantitative – reflects both the evolution of the field as well as how the
brain processes information – from very specific to diffuse patterns of
activation.Studies were included in this analysis regardless of the type of the
subjects employed to better reflect a wide variety of abilities. Some studies
used undifferentiated subjects while others included those with brain damage
and still others required the use of subjects with specifically defined skills.
Analyses
First,
we assessed the frequency of cited primary neural regions, which included the
frontal cortex, temporal cortex, parietal cortex, occipital cortex, cingulate
cortex, insular cortex, subcortical regions, and the cerebellum. We also ran a
secondary analysis on the primary regions that were most associated with each
of the intelligences (i.e., primary regions that represented at least 20% of
the primary neural citations). Within the top cited primary regions, we
identified the top sub-regions. All sub-regions that represented at least 20%
of a top primary neural regions were reported. Lastly, in some instances, a
third-level analysis was conducted to identify the important sub-regions within
a sub-region of a top primary neural region (e.g., frontal cortex à
prefrontal cortex à dorsomedial prefrontal cortex;
Appendix E). These second-level and third-level analyses are highlighted in the
text.
Results
The
following descriptions are highlights from an extensive dataset (see Appendix
F). Complete data and interpretations are available as supplemental material.
Interpersonal
The
interpersonal literature review identified 53 studies, including 111 citations
of primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of interpersonal
intelligence include social perception, interpersonal understanding, social
effectiveness, and leadership. Results from the analysis of the primary neural
regions can be found in Table 3 and Figure 1.
The
analysis of primary neural regions revealed that interpersonal intelligence was
most associated with the frontal cortex (43 citations). Secondary analyses more
specifically identified that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) accounted for the
large majority of frontal cortex citations (33/43 = 76.74%). A third-level
analysis revealed that the dorsolateral PFC was the dominant sub-region within
the PFC (8/33 = 24%).
Interpersonal
intelligence was also associated with the temporal cortex as revealed by 31
citations. Within the temporal cortex, the medial temporal lobe (9/31 = 29%),
amygdala (8/31 = 26%), and the superior temporal sulcus (7/31 = 23%) were the
predominantly cited sub-regions. Other notable regions associated with
Interpersonal intelligence included the cingulate cortex (12 citations), particularly
the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; 8/12 = 75%), and the parietal cortex (10
citations).
(Table 3 here)
(Figure 1 here)
Intrapersonal
The
intrapersonal literature review identified 73 studies, including 219 citations
of primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of intrapersonal
intelligence include self-awareness, self-regulation, executive functions, and
self-other management. Results from the analysis of the primary neural regions
can be found in Table 4 and Figure 2.
The
primary analysis revealed that Intrapersonal intelligence was most associated
with the frontal cortex (90 citations) – the large majority of which were
specific to the PFC (73/90 = 81%). A third-level analysis within the PFC
revealed the dorsomedial PFC (18/73 = 25%) and the lateral PFC (15/73 = 21%) as
major sub-regions.
The
primary analysis also identified the cingulate cortex (37 citations), temporal
cortex (36 citations), parietal cortex (25 citations), and subcortical regions
(20 citations). Within the cingulate cortex, dominant sub-regions included the
anterior cingulate cortex (27/37 = 73%). Within the temporal cortex, notable
sub-regions included the medial temporal lobe (9/36 = 25%), amygdala (8/36 =
22%), and anterior temporal cortex (8/36 = 22%). Within the parietal cortex,
the secondary analysis revealed that medial regions (10/25 = 40%) and inferior
regions (5/25 = 20%) were dominant. Lastly, within the subcortical regions, the
basal ganglia (10/20 = 50%) and brainstem (9/20 = 45%) were dominant. These
structures are associated with cognition, learning, reward management, and
unconscious memory (motor control).
(Table 4 here)
(Figure 2 here)
Visual-Spatial
The
visual-spatial intelligence literature review identified 37 studies, including
143 citations of primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of visual-spatial
intelligence include spatial cognition, working with objects, visual arts, and
spatial navigation. Results from the analysis of the primary neural regions can
be found in Table 5 and Figure 3.
The
primary analysis revealed the frontal cortex as the most associated with
visual-spatial intelligence (56 citations). Within the frontal cortex,
secondary analyses identified the motor cortex (21/56 = 38%) and PFC (17/56 =
31%) as most important. A third-level analysis within the motor cortex
highlighted the premotor cortex (12/21 = 57%) and the primary motor cortex
(5/21 = 24%) as dominant. Within the PFC, the third-level analysis revealed the
dorsolateral PC as most dominant (6/17 = 35%).
Furthermore,
the primary analysis identified the parietal cortex (29 citations) as the
second most dominant neural region for visual-spatial intelligence. Within the
parietal cortex, the intraparietal sulcus (7/29 = 24%) and superior parietal
lobule (7/29 = 24%) were notable sub-regions. A third-level analysis within the
superior parietal lobule identified the precuneus as dominant (3/7 = 43%).
Other
regions of interest included the temporal cortex (23 citations), including the
medial temporal lobe (8/23 = 35%). A third-level analysis within the medial
temporal lobe identified the hippocampus as the most dominant sub-region (4/8 =
50%). Furthermore, the primary analysis identified the occipital cortex (14
citations) as associated with visual-spatial intelligence, and a secondary
analysis within the occipital cortex specifically identified the primary visual
cortex as the most dominant sub-region (6/14 = 43%).
(Table 5 here)
(Figure 3 here)
Naturalist
The
naturalist literature review identified 25 studies, including 58 citations of
primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of naturalist intelligence
derived from MI theory as well as the neuroscience literatureincluded pattern
cognition, understanding living entities (including animals and plant life),
and science. Typical behaviors that were studied include perceiving animal
forms, motion, and vocalization; reading animal’s actions, intentions &
emotions; biological life detection; and taxonomic thinking. No studies were
found pertaining to understanding plant life. Results from the analysis of the
primary neural regions can be found in Table 6 and Figure 4.
Analysis
of the primary neural regions revealed that naturalist intelligence is most
associated with the temporal cortex (19 citations). Within the temporal cortex,
the secondary analysis identified the superior temporal sulcus (6/19 = 32%) and
amygdala (5/19 = 26%) as notable.
The
primary analysis also revealed subcortical neural regions (16 citations) as
important for naturalist intelligence. Notable subcortical regions included
regions of the brainstem (5/16 = 31%), the thalamus (5/16 = 31%), and the basal
ganglia (4/16 = 25%).
(Table 6 here)
(Figure 4 here)
Musical
The
musical literature review identified 42 studies, including 103 citations of
primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of musical intelligence
include music perception, music and emotions, and music production. Results
from the analysis of the primary neural regions can be found in Table 7 and
Figure 5.
Musical
intelligence was most associated with the frontal cortex (42 citations). Within
the frontal cortex, the motor cortex (31/42 = 74%) was the most dominant
sub-region. A third-level analysis revealed the premotor cortex (12/31 = 39%)
and the supplementary motor area (10/31 = 32%) as the most dominant
sub-regions.
The
next most frequently cited region was the temporal cortex (28 citations). A
secondary analysis revealed the most notable sub-region was the superior
temporal gyrus (23/28 = 82%), including the primary auditory cortex (19/23 =
83%, as revealed by a third-level analysis). Of other note, subcortical regions
(16 citations) were also implicated, primarily accounted for by the basal
ganglia (11/16 = 69%, as revealed by a third-level analysis).
(Table 7 here)
(Figure 5 here)
Kinesthetic
The
kinesthetic literature review identified 41 studies, including 142 citations of
primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of kinesthetic intelligence
included body awareness and control, whole body movement, dexterity, and other
types of movement (e.g., imitation, embodied cognition, gestures). Results from
the analysis of the primary neural regions can be found in Table 8 and Figure 6.
The
primary neural region analysis revealed the frontal cortex as most frequently
cited (61 citations). A secondary analysis revealed that the dominant sub-region
of the frontal cortex for kinesthetic intelligence was the motor cortex (46/61
= 75%). A third-level analysis further identified the primary motor cortex
(19/46 = 41%), premotor cortex (15/46 = 33%), and supplementary motor area
(9/46 = 20%) as dominant sub-regions.
Furthermore,
the primary analysis identified the parietal cortex as the next most associated
primary region (33 citations) within kinesthetic intelligence. Within the
parietal cortex, the posterior parietal cortex was associated with the most
citations (7/33 = 21%). Other regions of interest identified by the primary
analysis included subcortical regions (15 citations), including the basal
ganglia (11/15 = 73%, as indicated by secondary analysis) and thalamus (4/15 =
27%, as indicated by secondary analysis), as well as the cerebellum (13
citations).
(Table 8 here)
(Figure 6 here)
Linguistic
The
linguistic literature review identified 28 studies, including 124 citations of
primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of linguistic intelligence
included speech, reading, writing, and communication. Results from the analysis
of the primary neural regions can be found in Table 9 and Figure 7.
The
primary analysis revealed the temporal cortex (49 citations) as the most
dominant. Within the temporal cortex, the secondary analysis highlighted the
superior temporal gyrus (15/49 = 31%). Within the superior temporal gyrus, a
third-level analysis identified Wernicke’s Area as most prominent (5/15 =
33%).
The
primary analysis for linguistic intelligence also identified the frontal cortex
(33 citations) as a dominant region. The secondary analysis revealed the
inferior frontal gyrus (14/33 = 42%) as dominant within the frontal cortex.
Furthermore, a third-level analysis identified Broca’s Area within the inferior
frontal gyrus as dominant (13/14 = 93%). The secondary analysis with the frontal
cortex also identified the motor cortex (10/33 = 31%). Of note, the dominant
sub-regions of both the temporal cortex and frontal cortex have been identified
as critical for language processing, speech control, and speech production.
The
parietal cortex was also identified as an important region for Linguistic
intelligence (15 citations). A secondary analysis identified the inferior
parietal lobule (10/15 = 67%) as accounting for the most parietal cortex
citations, and a third-level analysis further identified both the supramarginal
gyrus (4/10 = 40%) and the angular gyrus (4/10 = 40%) as dominant sub-regions
of the inferior parietal lobule.
(Table 9 here)
(Figure 7 here)
Logical-Mathematical
The
logical-mathematical literature review identified 19 studies, including 71
citations of primary neural regions. The core cognitive units of logical-mathematical
intelligence were calculations and logical reasoning. Results from the analysis
of the primary neural regions can be found in Table 10 and Figure 8.
The
primary analysis revealed that logical-mathematical intelligence was most
associated with the frontal cortex (25 citations). Within the frontal cortex, logical-mathematical
intelligence was most associated with the PFC (11/25 = 44%) and the inferior
frontal gyrus (5/20 = 25%). A third-level analysis of PFC revealed the
dorsolateral PFC as the dominant sub-region (3/11 = 27%), and a third-level
analysis of the inferior frontal gyrus revealed Broca’s Area as the dominant
sub-region (4/5 = 80%). These regions have been associated with planning complex
behavior, judgment, decision-making, and language processing.
The
primary analysis also revealed that the parietal cortex (24 citations) was highly
associated with logical-mathematical intelligence. Within the parietal cortex,
logical-mathematical intelligence was primarily associated with the
intraparietal sulcus (7/24 = 42%) and inferior parietal lobule (7/24 = 42%). A
third-level analysis of the inferior parietal lobule revealed the angular gyrus
as the dominant sub-region (5/7 = 71%). Furthermore, the secondary-level
analysis of the parietal cortex identified the superior parietal lobule as a
dominant sub-region (5/24 = 21%). Within the superior parietal lobule, the
precuneus was most dominant (3/5 = 60%). These regions have been associated
with planning, working memory, numerical operations, attention, language, and
sensory interpretation.
To
a lesser extent, logical-mathematical intelligence was also associated with the
temporal cortex (15 citations), with the medial temporal lobe as a notable
sub-region (4/15 = 27%). It is noteworthy that neural structures associated
with logical-mathematical intelligence are also identified with general
intelligence.
(Table 10 here)
(Figure 8 here)
General
Intelligence
The general intelligence literature
review identified 24 studies for two cognitive units: analytical thinking and
verbal intelligence. From these studies, there were 100 citations for primary
regions, 132 for sub-regions and 47 for specific frontal structures.
General intelligence has four primary
regions that account for 93% of its citations –frontal cortex (33 citations), parietal
cortex (33 citations), temporal cortex (15 citations) and cingulate cortex (12
citations). There are very few citations within the occipital cortex (4
citations), subcortical regions (1 citation) and the cerebellum (1 citation).
Interestingly, these dominant regions are the same four primary regions in the
same order and nearly the same magnitude as cited for logical-mathematical
intelligence. This indicates that both general intelligence and logical-math
depend upon planning, complex reasoning, mental visualization, verbal
comprehension, and judgment (see Table 11).
Second-level analyses revealed that the
prefrontal cortex (12/33 = 36%) and the inferior frontal gyrus (6/33 = 18%)
were the most dominant sub-regions of the frontal cortex, while the inferior
parietal lobule (13/33 = 40%) was the most cited sub-region of the parietal
cortex. Within the temporal cortex, the superior temporal gyrus was the most
cited sub-region (3/15 = 20%), while the anterior cingulate cortex (8/12 = 67%)
was the most cited sub-region within the cingulate cortex.These are sub-regions
are largely associated with language, mathematical operations, complex
problem-solving, judgment, and impulse control. The only exception is the
anterior cingulate which is cited for general intelligence but not logical-math.
This region is thought to act as a gateway between the frontal and parietal
cortices and has beenassociated with early learning, decision making, empathy,
and managing the effort required for dealing with difficult problems.
The top three frontal structures cited
for general intelligence are also among the strongest for logical-mathematical
– prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus and posterior inferior frontal
gyrus. It is obvious that the frontal cortex is of fundamental importance to
doing both math and logical thinking. An interesting distinction is that the
intraparietal sulcus (IPS) is associated with logical-math but not general intelligence.
IPS appears to have a particular role in the understanding and processing of
numbers and numerosity. Additionally, it has been cited as a key structure for processing symbolic
numerical information, visuospatial working memory,and
theory of mind.
Taken together this constellation of
neural regions appears to be a primary processing system for abstracting information
and meaning from various kinds of sensory input requiring logical reasoning,
verbal comprehension and multi-step planning and execution(P-FIT) [2].Meta-analysis of neural research on general
intelligence conducted by [49, p. 24] extended the P-FIT model to“...propose an
updated neurocognitive model for the brain bases of intelligence that includes
insular cortex, posterior cingulate cortex and subcortical structures...”
(Table 11 here)
(Figure 9 here)
Summary of Results
Table
12 highlights the neural similarities and differences revealed by the primary
neural regional analysis. For each intelligence, the primary neural regions are
ranked based on the raw number of citations revealed by the literature review.
The columns display the eight intelligences, while the rows represent the rank
of each neural associate based on the frequency of citations associated with
each intelligence. In some cells, multiple neural regions are listed – this
simply reflects that those neural regions had identical citation frequencies.
Highlights of the sub-regional activation pattern per intelligence are
presented in Tables 13 – 16.
(Tables 12-16 here)
Discussion
A
variety of models have been proposed as to the neural underpinnings of
intelligence. One of the most accepted neural models for general intelligence (g) is called P-FIT** [1] which describes
g as being comprised primarily of the
parietal, frontal, and temporal regions. Other models have been offered for g as well [31, 32, 33, 33, 34 and others).
Despite the significant influence of MI theory on the field of education, no
study has directly and/or comprehensively assessed MI theory using
neuroscientific techniques. However, since the arrival of functional
neuroimaging in the 1990s,neuroscientists have extensively studied the neural
underpinnings of human cognition.
Of
present interest, such studies can be mapped onto each of the multiple
intelligences first outlined by Gardner [3,4] (see Table 1).For example,
aspects of cognition assessed within the neuroscience literature include
linguistic [35, 36], logical-mathematical [37, 38], musical [39, 40],
kinesthetic [41, 42], visual-spatial [43, 44], interpersonal [45, 46], and
intrapersonal [47, 48].
Several inter-related questions regarding the neuroscientific
evidence pertaining to eight hypothesized forms of intelligence and their
relationship with general intelligence were investigated. First, the review
revealed astrongcongruence among regions described by Gardner [3 ,4]and the
cognitive neuroscience literature that has accumulated since the advent of
functional neuroimaging.Such evidence provides support for MI theory.
A detailed examination of three levels of neural
analysis was employed in this review: primary, sub-regions and particular
structures within sub-regions. The primary neural region analysis divided the
brain into eight large neural regions (i.e., frontal cortex, parietal cortex,
temporal cortex, occipital cortex, cingulate cortex, insular cortex,
cerebellum, and subcortical structures) most frequently cited in the
literature. Six of the eight intelligences were most associated with the
frontal cortex, while the other two intelligences revealed the temporal cortex
as most dominant (see Table 12). The parietal and cingulate cortices were the
next most frequently associated with the intelligences. Alternatively, the
cerebellum and insular cortex were never ranked within the top three most
associated neural regions for any of the eight intelligences.
These data highlight the commonalities among the
eight intelligences. However, the primary region analysis largely identified
distinct neural configurationsfor each intelligence (see Table 12). For
example, none of intelligences shared the same top three ranked regions.
Furthermore, the frequency of citations for each of the primary neural regions
cited for each intelligence varies a great deal. The figures depicting the
distribution of citation frequency are compelling evidence for these distinct
regional patterns.
Secondary and tertiary neural sub-region analyses
were conducted to identify the specific neural structures within the primary
neural regions associated with each intelligence. Secondary sub-region analyses
reveal which particular regions are most associated with each of the
intelligences. For example, the frontal cortex accounted for approximately 40%
of citations for both musical and intrapersonal, which may suggest a neural
similarity. However, secondary analysis revealed that approximately 75% of the
frontal cortex citations were specific to the motor cortex for musical
intelligence, while approximately 81% of the frontal cortex citations were
specific to the prefrontal cortex for intrapersonal intelligence. Critically,
these two sub-regions of the PFC are quite distinct in function.
A third-level examination of specific structures
within sub-regions describes a distinct configuration of structures responsible
for processing each of the eight intelligences. For example, the visual-spatial
intelligence is associated with the parietal cortex (primary level) and
intraparietal and superior parietal lobule (sub-regions) and also the precuneus
(third-level). This example, and many others,
highlights the necessity for including neural sub-region analyses to fully
describe the neural substrates for each intelligence. For more extensive data
on sub-region level differences, readers
should refer to Appendix F and to the supplemental dataset.
Based on the detailed analysis of over 318neuroscience
studies it appears there is robustevidence that each of the eight intelligences
possesses its own unique neural architecture. There are also theoretically
consistent commonalities among related intelligences. Understanding these
unique configurations and commonalitiesprovides insight into how the brain
processes a full range of intellectual products and performances.
Finally, how well do these neural architectures
compare to the neural correlates for general intelligence?As predicted by MI
theory, the neural correlates for general intelligence are nearly identical to
those responsible for processing the logical-mathematical and linguistic
intelligences. The association is stronger for logical-mathematical than it is
for linguistic. This may be because most neuroscientists use logical
problem-solving tasks (e.g., Raven’s Progressive Matrices) as measures for g. Likewise, measures of verbal ability
emphasize convergent problem-solving.
Limitations and Future
Directions
Several
limitations to this analysis should be noted. First, by necessity the
interpretation of the data from over 318 studies had to be conducted with
broad-brush strokes that accentuate the frequency of neural citations for a
specified class of cognitive behaviors. This approach can neglect or minimize
the importance of a particular structure or even multi-region activation
patterns and conductivity efficiencies. Also, instances of neural inhibition
were missing from these accounts, which can play a crucial role in cognition
(e.g., reduced critical thinking in the service of divergent thinking). A
review of the neural data for each intelligence by an expert review panel would
go a long way toward evaluating and clarifying the neural architecture for the
intelligences.
Second,
this analysis has concentrated on the eight broad MI constructs, but perhaps of
equal importance in the formulation of a robust scientific theory are the core
cognitive units within each intelligence. These core units represent specific
instances of skill and ability that require a fine-grained neural analysis
within an overarching theoretical framework. This is analogousto the
identification of working memory, attentional control and language processing
as components of general intelligence. Both statistical and expert reviews will
serve to clarify the neural and specific characteristics of these cognitive
units.
Third,
an essential feature of any theory of intelligence is that it helps us to
understand the differences among ability group levels[49]. A challenging next
step for this investigation would be to describe key neural differences among
impaired, typical and expert individuals for each intelligence (or components
and combinations of intelligences).
Fourth,
the relationship among the eight intelligences and various information
processing capacities (e.g., attention, concentration, cognitive control and
memory, etc.) needs further clarification.This could also provide an
opportunity to determine how logical problem-solving is related to all eight
intelligences. This study has also revealed the possibility that there are
several general cognitive abilities that are essential elements of MI theory –
Insight / Intuition, Aesthetic Judgment and Creativity – that may be comparable
to general intelligence. These capacities have neural correlates described in
the literature, e.g., Qui, et al. [50], Fink, et al. [51] and Calvo-Merino, B. et al. [52]. A preliminary
analysis is forthcoming.
This investigation focused on data that
describes the localization of regions in the brain that are activated by
intelligent performances in each area. As advocated by Basten, et al. [49, p.
27] such an analysis “...can only be a
first step in understanding how intelligence evolves from the brain...Only the
integration of the current localization-focused results with neural
network-based investigations of dynamic interactions in the brain may finally
enable us to understand how the brain supports intelligent performance.”
Studies of inter-regional resting-state
functional connectivity (rsFC) bySadaghiani [53] and many others have
highlighted the importance of recognizing the influence of individual
differences on task performance. A future review of rsFC research may shed
light on questions regarding the influence of individual differences on
academic achievement and life success. Furthermore, the neural overlap among
intelligences needs further clarification as possible focus points for
leveraging achievement in a particular skill by using a strength to enhance development.
These findings could provide valuable information for guiding instructional
interventions that are “personalized” to take into account each learner’s
unique strengths for the direct improvement of deficits [13, 54].
Conclusions
This investigation uncovered a wealth
of neuroscience evidence that describes in great detail the neural
underpinnings of skills associated with both general intelligence and the eight
multiple intelligences. To describe MI and g
as mutually incompatible entities seems to be more of a cultural preference
rather than a conclusion derived from the neuroscientific evidence. There are
important points of confluence that might serve as a basis for a comprehensive
theory of educational cognitive neuroscience. Due to theoretical disagreements
and cultural biases, whether MI theory can serve as an effective interface
between neuroscience and education remains an open question.Describing how the brain
works is scientifically challenging but neuroscience is making great strides. It
may prove to be an even harder task to create a Y-shaped bridge that merges IQ
with MI to channel our energies into the “art of teaching” so that all students
can develop their unique potential along with their academic skills.
Notes:
*To qualify as an intelligence, each set of
abilities has to fair reasonably well in meeting eight criteria as specified in
Frames of Mind ([3, p. 62 – 67]:
1- identifiable cerebral systems
2- evolutionary history and plausibility
3- core set of operations
4- meaning encoded in a symbol system
5- a distinct developmental history &
mastery
6- savants, prodigies and exceptional people
7- evidence from experimental psychology
8- psychometric findings
Definition: Intelligence is a biopsychological potential
to process information that can be activated
in a cultural setting to solve problems or create products that are of
value in a culture. Intelligence
Reframed[4]
** Haier and Jung [2, p. 173]describe a widely
distributed neural network model that underpins intelligence called the
Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT)involving thefrontal lobes, parietal,
temporal and occipital cortices.
“The P-FIT
recognizes that our species gathers and processes information predominantly
through auditory and/or visual means, usually in combination; thus, particular
brainregions within the temporal and occipital lobes are critical to early
processing of sensoryinformation: the extrastriate cortex (BAs 18, 19) and
fusiform gyrus (BA 37), involving recognition and subsequent imagery and/or
elaboration of visual input, and Wernicke’s area (BA 22), involving analysis
and/or elaboration of syntax of auditory information. This basic sensory
processing is then fed forward to the parietal cortex, predominantly the
supramarginal (BA 40), inferior parietal (BA 7), and angular (BA 39) gyri,
wherein structural symbolism and/or abstraction of the current set to
alternative cognitive sets are generated and elaborated. The parietal cortex
interacts with frontal regions (i.e., BAs 6, 9, 10, 45–47), which serve to
hypothesis test various solutions to a given problem. Once the best solution
emerges, the anterior cingulate (BA 32) is engaged to constrain response
selection as well as inhibition of other competing responses. This process is
critically dependent upon the fidelity of underlying white matter needed to
facilitate rapid and error free transmission of data from posterior to frontal
brain regions
References
[1] Jung, R. E.,
& Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of
intelligence:
Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences,30,
135–187.
[2] Haier, R. & Jung,
R. (2008) Brain imaging studies of intelligence and creativity: What is the
picture for education?Roeper Review, 30:3, 171-180, DOI:10.1080/02783190802199347
[3] Gardner, H.
(1983,1993). Frames of mind: The theory
of multiple intelligences.
New York:Basic Books.
[4] Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence
reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century.
New
York:Basic
Books.
[5] Herrnstein, R.,
& Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve. New York: Free Press.
[6] Waterhouse, L.
(2006b). Inadequate evidence for multiple intelligences, Mozart effect, and
emotional
intelligence theories. Educational
Psychologist, 41, 247–255.
[7] Sternberg, R.
J. (1996). Successful intelligence. New York: Simon & Schuster.
[8] Traub, J. &
Gardner, H. (1999). A debate on multiple intelligences. The Dana Foundation,
[9] Waterhouse, L.
(2006a). Multiple intelligences, the Mozart effect, and emotional intelligence:
A critical review.Educational Psychologist, 41, 207–225.
[10] Jensen, A.
(1999). The g factor: the science of mental ability. Psycoloquy. American
[11] Visser, B., Ashton,
M. & Vernon, P. (2006). Beyond g: Puttingmultiple intelligences theory
tothe test. Intelligence, 34, 487–502.
[12] Gerin,
B. and Fien, H. (2016). Translating the neuroscience of physical activity to
education.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education.
5, 12–19.
[13] Hale, J.B., et al.
(2016). Reconciling individual differences with collective needs: The
juxtaposition of
socialpolitical and neuroscience perspectives on remdiation and compensation of
student skill deficits. Trends in
Neuroscience and Education. 5, 41–51.
[14] Gottfredson, L. (Summer, 2004). Schools and the g factor.Wilson Quarterly. Washington,
DC.The
Woodrow Wilson International Center for Scholars.
[15] White, J. (1988). Do Howard
Gardner’s multiple intelligences add up? London: Institute
OfEducation, University
of London.
[16] Willingham,
D.T. (2004) Reframing the mind. Retrieved 10-1-04 from
[17]Sternberg, R.J.
(1988). The triarchic mind. New York:
Viking.
[18]Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional
intelligence. New York: Bantam.
[19] Salovey, P.,
& Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence.Imagination, Cognition,
and
Personality, 9, 185–211.
[20] Guilford, J.
L. (1954). The nature of human intelligence.New York: McGraw-Hill.
[21] Thurstone,
L.L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
[22]Kolb, A. Y.,
& Kolb, D. A. (2005). The Kolb learning style inventory – version 3.1:
2005
[23]Chen, J.,
Moran, S. & Gardner, H. (Ed.) (2009). Multiple
intelligences around the world.
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
[24] Brody, N.
(1992). Intelligence. New York:
Academic Press.
[25] Chomsky, N.
(2009). A conversation about multiple
intelligences / an interview with Noam
Chomsky. inMI at 25: Assessing the impact and future of multiple intelligences
for teaching and learning. New York:
Teachers College Press.
[26] Bruer, J. T.
(1997). Education and the brain: A bridge too far. EducationalResearcher,
26,
4-16.
[27] Goswami, U.,
&Szucs, D. (2011). Educational neuroscience: Developmentalmechanisms:
Towards a conceptual framework.NeuroImage, 57, 651–658.
[28] Immordino-Yang,
M.H. &Gotlieb, R. (2016). Embodied brains, social minds, cultural
meaning: Integrating neuroscientific
and educational research on social-affective
development (Manuscript accepted). American Educational Research Journal:
Centennial Issue.
[29] Tokuhama-Espinosa,
T. (2010). Mind, Brain, and Education
Science: A Comprehensive
Guide
to the New Brain-Based Teaching. New York: WW Norton.
[30] Schwartz, M. (June, 2015). Mind, brain and
education: A decade of evolution. Mind,
Brain and Education.Vol. 9, 2.
[31] Barbey, K., Colom,
R., Solomon, J.,Krueger, F., Forbes, C. Grafman, J., (2012). An
integrativearchitecture for general intelligence and executive function
revealed by lesion
mapping. Brain, DOI:
10.1093/brain/aws021
[32] Duncan, J.et al. (2000).A neural basis for
general intelligence.Science 289, 457.
DOI:
10.1126/science.289.5478.457
[33] Grabner, R. et
al. (2006). Superior performance and
neural efficiency: The impact of
intelligence and expertise. Brain
Research Bulletin 69, 422–439.
[34] Neubauer, A.
et al. (2005). Intelligence and neural efficiency: Further evidence of the
influence of task content and sex on the brain–IQ relationship. Cognitive Brain
Research, 25, 217–225.
[35] McCandliss,
B. andNoble, K., ( 2003). The development of reading impairment:
Acognitive neuroscience model.
Mental Retardation And Developmental Disabilities.
Research
Reviews9: 196–205.
[36]Price, C. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20
years of PET and fMRI studies of
heard speech,spoken language and reading. NeuroImage62.816–847.
[37] Barbey, AK and
Barsalou,LW. (2009). Reasoning and problem solving: Models.
Encyclopedia of Neuroscience (2009),
vol. 8, pp. 35-43.
[38] Dehaene, S., Molko, M.,Cohen, L. and Wilson, A. (2004).Arithmetic and the brain.Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14:218–224.
[39] Gaser,
C. &Schlaug, G. (2003).Brain structures differ betweenmusicians and non-
musicians. J. Neurosci. 23, 9240–9245.
[40] Zatorre, R., and
Salimpoor, V. (2013). From perception to pleasure: Music and its neural
substrates.
PNAS,
vol. 110, suppl.
[41] Brown S,
Martinez MJ, Parsons LM (2006): The neural basis ofhuman dance. Cereb Cortex
16:1157–1167.
[42] Jantzen,
K., Steinberg, F. and Kelso, J. (2008).Coordination
dynamics of large-scale
neuralcircuitry underlying rhythmic sensorimotor behavior
Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience 21:12, pp. 2420–2433.
[43] Aziz-Zadeh,L., Liew,S., and Dandekar, F. (2013). Exploring the neural correlates of
visual creativitySCAN 8, 475 – 480.
[44] Cui, X. (2007). Vividness of mental
imagery: Individual variability can be measured
objectively.
Vision
Research 47 474–478.
[45] Decety, J, Skelly, L., Kiehl, K.A. (2013).
Brain response to empathy-eliciting
scenarios involving pain in incarcerated
individuals with psychopathy.JAMA
Psychiatry.70(6):638-645.
doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.27.
[46] Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural
basis of social knowledge.Annu Rev Psychol.
2009 ; 60: 693–716.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514
[47] Damasio, A.(2003).
Mental self: The person within. Nature 423
(6937), 227.
[48] Northoff, G.,Heinzel,
A., de Greck, M.,Bermpohl, F.,Dobrowolny, H.,Panksepp, J.
(2006), Self-referential processing in our brain—A
meta-analysis of imaging studies on
the self. NeuroImage.
Vol. 31 Issue 1, p440-457. 18p.
[49] Basten, U., Hilger, K. &Fiebach, C. J. (2015).
Where smart brains are different: A
quantitative meta-analysis of
functional and structural brain imaging studies on
[50] Qiu, J.
et al. (2008) The neural basis of insight problem solving: An event-related
potential
study. Brain and
Cognition 68 (100–106).
[51] Fink, A. et al. (2007). Creativity meets
neuroscience: Experimental tasks for the
neuroscientific study of creative
thinking. ScienceDirect.42, 68 – 76.
[52] Calvo-Merino, B. et al. (2007). Towards a
sensorimotor aesthetics of performing art.
Consciousness
and Cognition 17, 911- 922.
[53]Sadaghiani, S. et
al. (2010). The relation of ongoing brain activity, evoked neural responses,
and cognition. Front. Syst Neuroscience 4, 20.
[54] Chen, J., Krechevsky, M. &Viens, J.
(1998). Building on children's strengths: The
experience of project spectrum.
New York: Teachers College Press
Tables
Table
1.
The Neural Correlates of the Multiple Intelligences Originally Identified by
Gardner
Intelligences
|
Neural
Regions
|
Interpersonal
|
Frontal lobes as integrating station, limbic system
|
Intrapersonal
|
Frontal lobe system
|
Logical-Mathematical
|
Left parietal lobes
& adjacent temporal & occipital association areas, left hemisphere
for verbal naming, right hemisphere for spatial organization, frontal system
for planning and goal setting
|
Linguistic
|
Broca’s area in left inferior frontal cortex, Wernicke’s area in the
left temporal lobe, lateral sulcus loop inferior parietal lobule
|
Spatial
|
Right parietal posterior, occipital lobe
|
Naturalist
|
Left parietal lobe for discriminating living from non-living entities
|
Musical
|
Right anterior temporal and frontal lobes
|
Kinesthetic
|
Cerebral motor
strip, thalamus, basal ganglia, cerebellum
|
Source.[3] Frames of Mind (1983, 1993), [4] Intelligence Reframed (1999).
Table 2.Details of
Neuroscience Literature Review for Multiple Intelligences.
Intelligence
|
Search terms
|
N
|
Years
|
Citations
|
Original Core Cognitive Units
|
Revised Core Cognitive Units
|
Linguistic
|
Verbal
skill
Reading
Writing
Speaking
Rhetoric
|
28
|
1998–2015
|
362
|
-Language comprehension
-Spoken language
-Writing
-Reading
|
-Speech
-Reading
-Writing
-Multimodal Communication of Meaning
|
Logical-mathematical
|
Reasoning
Calculations
Math
skill
Abstraction
Meaning
making
|
19
|
2000–2013
|
177
|
-Calculations
-Logical reasoning
-Problem Solving
|
-Mathematical Reasoning
-Logical Reasoning
|
Musical
|
Vocal
/ Singing
Instrumental
ability
Musical
appreciation
Improvisation
Music emotions
|
42
|
1985-2013
|
288
|
-Perceiving pitch, melody, harmony,
timbre and rhythm
-Vocal singing
-Emotional aspects of music
-Instrumental music
-Perception of both music and the
sounds of human language
|
-Music Perception
-Music and Emotions
-Music Production
|
Kinesthetic
|
Large
motor movement
Fine
motor
Dexterity
Tool
use
Eye
Hand coordination
Dance
Athletics
|
41
|
1977-2015
|
349
|
-Fine motor
movements
-Large motor movements
-Expressive Movements
-Motor memory
|
-Body Awareness/Control
-Whole Body Movement
-Dexterity
-Symbolic Movement
|
Spatial
|
Mental
visualization
Imagination
Spatial
orientation
|
37
|
1978–2015
|
385
|
-Spatial
-Awareness
-Working w/Objects
-Art Perception
-Art Production
|
-Spatial Cognition
-Working with Objects
-Visual Arts
-Spatial Navigation
|
Interpersonal
|
Empathy
Theory
of mind
Interpersonal
perspective taking
Leadership
|
53
|
1989–2013
|
294
|
-Empathy
-Understanding Others
-Leadership
-Facilitator / Caregiver
|
-Social Perception
-Interpersonal Understanding
-Social Effectiveness
-Leadership
|
Intrapersonal
|
Metacognition
Emotional
intelligence
Self-management
Impulse
control
|
73
|
1998-2014
|
627
|
-Self Understanding
-Metacognition
-Emotional Management
|
-Self-Awareness
-Self-Regulation
-Executive Functions
-Self-Other Management
|
Naturalist
|
Understanding
animals
Plant
care
Science
Classification
|
25
|
1969–2015
|
172
|
-Understanding Animals
-Understanding Plants
-Pattern recognition
-Science
|
-Pattern Cognition
-Understanding Living Entities
-Understanding Animals
-Understanding Plant Life
-Science
|
|
Totals
|
318
|
|
2,654
|
|
|
Table 3. Interpersonal:
Analysis of Primary Neural Regions
Interpersonal
|
||
Primary Neural
Region
|
Citations
(N=111)
|
% of Citations
|
Frontal Cortex
|
43
|
38.74
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
31
|
27.93
|
Cingulate
Cortex
|
12
|
10.81
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
10
|
9.01
|
Insular Cortex
|
6
|
5.41
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
4
|
3.60
|
Subcortical
Structures
|
4
|
3.60
|
Cerebellum
|
1
|
0.90
|
Table 4. Intrapersonal:
Analysis of Primary Neural Regions
Intrapersonal
|
||
Primary Neural
Regions
|
Citations
(N=219)
|
% of Citations
|
Frontal Cortex
|
90
|
41.10
|
Cingulate
Cortex
|
37
|
16.89
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
36
|
16.44
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
25
|
11.42
|
Subcortical Structures
|
20
|
9.13
|
Insular Cortex
|
9
|
4.11
|
Cerebellum
|
2
|
0.91
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
0
|
0.00
|
Table 5. Visual-spatial:
Analysis of Primary Neural Regions
Spatial
|
||
Primary Neural
Regions
|
Citations
(N=143)
|
% of Citations
|
Frontal Cortex
|
56
|
39.16
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
29
|
20.28
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
23
|
16.08
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
14
|
9.79
|
Subcortical
Structures
|
12
|
8.39
|
Cerebellum
|
5
|
3.50
|
Cingulate
Cortex
|
3
|
2.10
|
Insular Cortex
|
1
|
0.70
|
Table 6. Naturalist:
Analysis of Primary Neural Regions
Naturalist
|
||
Primary Neural
Regions
|
Citations
(N=58)
|
% of Citations
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
19
|
32.76
|
Subcortical
Structures
|
16
|
27.59
|
Frontal Cortex
|
7
|
12.07
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
7
|
12.07
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
7
|
12.07
|
Cerebellum
|
1
|
1.72
|
Insular Cortex
|
1
|
1.72
|
Cingulate
Cortex
|
0
|
0.00
|
Table
7.
Musical: Analysis of Primary Neural Regions
Musical
|
||
Primary Neural
Regions
|
Citations
(N=103)
|
% of Citations
|
Frontal Cortex
|
42
|
40.78
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
28
|
27.18
|
Subcortical
Structures
|
16
|
15.53
|
Cerebellum
|
10
|
9.71
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
5
|
4.85
|
Insular Cortex
|
2
|
1.94
|
Cingulate
Cortex
|
0
|
0.00
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
0
|
0.00
|
Table 8.
Kinesthetic:Analysis of Primary Neural Regions.
Kinesthetic
|
||
Primary Neural
Regions
|
Citations
(N=142)
|
% of Citations
|
Frontal Cortex
|
61
|
42.96
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
33
|
23.24
|
Subcortical
Structures
|
15
|
10.56
|
Cerebellum
|
13
|
9.15
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
8
|
5.63
|
Cingulate
Cortex
|
6
|
4.23
|
Insular Cortex
|
5
|
3.52
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
1
|
0.70
|
Table 9.Linguistic:
Analysis of Primary Neural Regions
Linguistic
|
||
Primary Neural
Regions
|
Citations
(N=124)
|
% of Citations
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
49
|
39.52
|
Frontal Cortex
|
33
|
26.61
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
15
|
12.10
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
9
|
7.26
|
Subcortical
Structures
|
9
|
7.26
|
Cerebellum
|
5
|
4.03
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
2
|
1.61
|
Insular Cortex
|
2
|
1.61
|
Table 10.
Logical-Mathematical: Analysis of Primary Neural Regions
Logical/Math
|
||
Primary Neural
Regions
|
Citations
(N=71)
|
% of Citations
|
Frontal Cortex
|
25
|
35.21
|
Parietal
Cortex
|
24
|
33.80
|
Temporal
Cortex
|
15
|
21.13
|
Cingulate
Cortex
|
5
|
7.04
|
Insular Cortex
|
1
|
1.41
|
Occipital
Cortex
|
1
|
1.41
|
Cerebellum
|
0
|
0.00
|
Subcortical
Structures
|
0
|
0.00
|
Table 11. Neural
Highlights for General Intelligence
General Intelligence Neural Highlights
|
|||||
Main
|
%
|
Sub-regions
|
%
|
Frontal Structures
|
Ct.
|
Frontal
|
33
|
Inferior Parietal
Lobule
|
10
|
Prefrontal Cortex
|
12
|
Parietal
|
33
|
Prefrontal Cortex
|
9
|
Inferior Frontal
Gyrus
|
6
|
Temporal
|
15
|
Anterior
Cingulate
|
6
|
Posterior
Inferior Frontal Gyrus
|
4
|
Cingulate
|
12
|
Inferior Frontal
Gyrus
|
5
|
Broca’s Area
|
4
|
|
|
Supramarginal
Gyrus (Angular Gyrus)
|
4
|
|
|
Total
|
100
|
Total
|
132
|
Total
|
47
|
Table 12.Analysis of Primary Neural Regions: Summary
of Relative Citation Frequencies.
|
Intelligences
|
||||||||
Interpersonal
|
Intrapersonal
|
Logical-Math
|
Linguistic
|
Spatial
|
Naturalist
|
Musical
|
Kinesthetic
|
||
Rank
|
1
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
|
2
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Subcortical
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Parietal Cortex
|
|
3
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
Parietal Cortex
Occipital Cortex
|
Subcortical
|
Subcortical
|
|
4
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
Occipital Cortex
Subcortical
|
Occipital Cortex
|
-
|
Cerebellum
|
Cerebellum
|
|
5
|
Insular Cortex
|
Subcortical
|
Occipital Cortex
Insular Cortex
|
-
|
Subcortical
|
-
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Temporal Cortex
|
|
6
|
Occipital Cortex
Subcortical
|
Insular Cortex
|
-
|
Cerebellum
|
Cerebellum
|
Insular Cortex
Cerebellum
|
Insular Cortex
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
|
7
|
-
|
Cerebellum
|
Subcortical
Cerebellum
|
Cingulate Cortex
Insular Cortex
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
-
|
Occipital
Cingulate Cortex
|
Insular Cortex
|
|
8
|
Cerebellum
|
-
|
-
|
-
|
Insular Cortex
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
-
|
Occipital Cortex
|
Table 13.Interpersonal and
Intrapersonal: A review of top neural structures
|
Interpersonal
|
Intrapersonal
|
|||
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
||
Rank
|
1
|
Frontal Cortex
|
PFC
|
Frontal Cortex
|
PFC
|
2
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Medial Temporal Lobe
Amygdala
Superior Temporal Sulcus
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
ACC
|
|
3
|
Cingulate Cortex
|
ACC
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Medial Temporal Lobe
Anterior Temporal Lobe
Amygdala
|
|
4
|
Parietal Cortex
|
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Medial Parietal Cortex
Inferior Parietal Cortex
|
|
5
|
|
|
Subcortical
|
Basal Ganglia
Brainstem
|
Table 14.Logical-Mathematical
and Linguistic: A review of top neural structures
|
Logical-Mathematical
|
Linguistic
|
|||
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
||
Rank
|
1
|
Frontal Cortex
|
PFC
Inferior Frontal Gyrus
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Superior Temporal Gyrus
|
2
|
Parietal
|
Intraparietal Sulcus
Inferior Parietal Lobule
Angular Gyrus
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Broca’s Area
Motor Cortex
|
|
3
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Medial Temporal Lobe
|
Parietal
|
Inferior Parietal Lobule
Supramarginal Gyrus
Angular Gyrus
|
Table 15.Spatial and
Naturalist: A review of top neural structures
|
Spatial
|
Naturalist
|
|||
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
||
Rank
|
1
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Motor Cortex
PFC
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Superior Temporal Sulcus
Amygdala
|
2
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Intraparietal Sulcus
Superior Parietal Lobe
|
Subcortical Structures
|
Brainstem
Thalamus
Basal Ganglia
|
|
3
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Medial Temporal Lobe
|
Frontal Cortex
|
-
|
|
4
|
Occipital Cortex
|
-
|
Occipital Cortex
|
-
|
|
5
|
-
|
-
|
Parietal Cortex
|
-
|
Table 16.Musical and Kinesthetic:
A review of top neural structures
|
Musical
|
Kinesthetic
|
|||
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
Primary
|
Sub-regions
|
||
Rank
|
1
|
Frontal
|
Motor Cortex
|
Frontal Cortex
|
Motor Cortex
Primary Motor
Premotor
Supplementary Motor
|
2
|
Temporal Cortex
|
Superior Temporal Sulcus
Primary Auditory Cortex
|
Parietal Cortex
|
Posterior Parietal Cortex
|
|
3
|
Subcortical Structures
|
Basal Ganglia
|
Subcortical
|
Basal Ganglia
Thalamus
|
|
4
|
-
|
-
|
Cerebellum
|
-
|
Σχόλια
Δημοσίευση σχολίου